Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
: 20 | 50 | 100
1 - 20 de 6.314
1.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 10040, 2024 05 02.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38693189

Investigation of visual illusions helps us understand how we process visual information. For example, face pareidolia, the misperception of illusory faces in objects, could be used to understand how we process real faces. However, it remains unclear whether this illusion emerges from errors in face detection or from slower, cognitive processes. Here, our logic is straightforward; if examples of face pareidolia activate the mechanisms that rapidly detect faces in visual environments, then participants will look at objects more quickly when the objects also contain illusory faces. To test this hypothesis, we sampled continuous eye movements during a fast saccadic choice task-participants were required to select either faces or food items. During this task, pairs of stimuli were positioned close to the initial fixation point or further away, in the periphery. As expected, the participants were faster to look at face targets than food targets. Importantly, we also discovered an advantage for food items with illusory faces but, this advantage was limited to the peripheral condition. These findings are among the first to demonstrate that the face pareidolia illusion persists in the periphery and, thus, it is likely to be a consequence of erroneous face detection.


Illusions , Humans , Female , Male , Adult , Illusions/physiology , Young Adult , Visual Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Face/physiology , Facial Recognition/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
2.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 3141, 2024 Apr 23.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38653975

Brightness illusions are a powerful tool in studying vision, yet their neural correlates are poorly understood. Based on a human paradigm, we presented illusory drifting gratings to mice. Primary visual cortex (V1) neurons responded to illusory gratings, matching their direction selectivity for real gratings, and they tracked the spatial phase offset between illusory and real gratings. Illusion responses were delayed compared to real gratings, in line with the theory that processing illusions requires feedback from higher visual areas (HVAs). We provide support for this theory by showing a reduced V1 response to illusions, but not real gratings, following HVAs optogenetic inhibition. Finally, we used the pupil response (PR) as an indirect perceptual report and showed that the mouse PR matches the human PR to perceived luminance changes. Our findings resolve debates over whether V1 neurons are involved in processing illusions and highlight the involvement of feedback from HVAs.


Neurons , Optogenetics , Photic Stimulation , Primary Visual Cortex , Animals , Neurons/physiology , Primary Visual Cortex/physiology , Mice , Male , Humans , Female , Visual Perception/physiology , Illusions/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Pupil/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Cortex/cytology
3.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38656862

Illusory directional sensations are generated through asymmetric vibrations applied to the fingertips and have been utilized to induce upper-limb motions in the rehabilitation and training of patients with visual impairment. However, its effects on motor control remain unclear. This study aimed to verify the effects of illusory directional sensations on wrist motion. We conducted objective and subjective evaluations of wrist motion during a motor task, while inducing an illusory directional sensation that was congruent or incongruent with wrist motion. We found that, when motion and illusory directional sensations were congruent, the sense of agency for motion decreased. This indicates an induction sensation of the hand being moved by the illusion. Interestingly, although no physical force was applied to the hand, the angular velocity of the wrist was higher in the congruent condition than that in the no-stimulation condition. The angular velocity of the wrist and electromyography signals of the agonist muscles were weakly positively correlated, suggesting that the participants may have increased their wrist velocity. In other words, the congruence between the direction of motion and illusory directional sensation induced the sensation of the hand being moved, even though the participants' wrist-motion velocity increased. This phenomenon can be explained by the discrepancy between the sensation of active motion predicted by the efferent copy, and that of actual motion caused by the addition of the illusion. The findings of this study can guide the design of novel rehabilitation methods.


Electromyography , Illusions , Movement , Vibration , Wrist , Humans , Illusions/physiology , Male , Female , Wrist/physiology , Young Adult , Adult , Movement/physiology , Hand/physiology , Healthy Volunteers , Motion , Proprioception/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Sensation/physiology
4.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 9302, 2024 04 23.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38654060

We capitalized on the respiratory bodily illusion that we discovered in a previous study and called 'Embreathment' where we showed that breathing modulates corporeal awareness in men. Despite the relevance of the issue, no such studies are available in women. To bridge this gap, we tested whether the synchronization of avatar-participant respiration patterns influenced females' bodily awareness. We collected cardiac and respiratory interoceptive measures, administered body (dis)satisfaction questionnaires, and tracked participants' menstrual cycles via a mobile app. Our approach allowed us to characterize the 'Embreathment' illusion in women, and explore the relationships between menstrual cycle, interoception and body image. We found that breathing was as crucial as visual appearance in eliciting feelings of ownership and held greater significance than any other cue with respect to body agency in both women and men. Moreover, a positive correlation between menstrual cycle days and body image concerns, and a negative correlation between interoceptive sensibility and body dissatisfaction were found, confirming that women's body dissatisfaction arises during the last days of menstrual cycle and is associated with interoception. These findings have potential implications for corporeal awareness alterations in clinical conditions like eating disorders and schizophrenia.


Awareness , Body Image , Illusions , Interoception , Virtual Reality , Humans , Female , Body Image/psychology , Adult , Illusions/physiology , Young Adult , Interoception/physiology , Male , Menstrual Cycle/physiology , Menstrual Cycle/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Respiration , Body Dissatisfaction/psychology
5.
PLoS One ; 19(4): e0295342, 2024.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38568979

It has been shown that observing a face being touched or moving in synchrony with our own face increases self-identification with the former which might alter both cognitive and affective processes. The induction of this phenomenon, termed enfacement illusion, has often relied on laboratory tools that are unavailable to a large audience. However, digital face filters applications are nowadays regularly used and might provide an interesting tool to study similar mechanisms in a wider population. Digital filters are able to render our faces in real time while changing important facial features, for example, rendering them more masculine or feminine according to normative standards. Recent literature using full-body illusions has shown that participants' own gender identity shifts when embodying a different gendered avatar. Here we studied whether participants' filtered faces, observed while moving in synchrony with their own face, may induce an enfacement illusion and if so, modulate their gender identity. We collected data from 35 female and 33 male participants who observed a stereotypically gender mismatched version of themselves either moving synchronously or asynchronously with their own face on a screen. Our findings showed a successful induction of the enfacement illusion in the synchronous condition according to a questionnaire addressing the feelings of ownership, agency and perceived similarity. However, we found no evidence of gender identity being modulated, neither in explicit nor in implicit measures of gender identification. We discuss the distinction between full-body and facial processing and the relevance of studying widely accessible devices that may impact the sense of a bodily self and our cognition, emotion and behaviour.


Illusions , Touch Perception , Humans , Male , Female , Gender Identity , Self Concept , Touch
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(17): e2400086121, 2024 Apr 23.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38621132

Vision can provide useful cues about the geometric properties of an object, like its size, distance, pose, and shape. But how the brain merges these properties into a complete sensory representation of a three-dimensional object is poorly understood. To address this gap, we investigated a visual illusion in which humans misperceive the shape of an object due to a small change in one eye's retinal image. We first show that this illusion affects percepts of a highly familiar object under completely natural viewing conditions. Specifically, people perceived their own rectangular mobile phone to have a trapezoidal shape. We then investigate the perceptual underpinnings of this illusion by asking people to report both the perceived shape and pose of controlled stimuli. Our results suggest that the shape illusion results from distorted cues to object pose. In addition to yielding insights into object perception, this work informs our understanding of how the brain combines information from multiple visual cues in natural settings. The shape illusion can occur when people wear everyday prescription spectacles; thus, these findings also provide insight into the cue combination challenges that some spectacle wearers experience on a regular basis.


Illusions , Humans , Brain , Cues
7.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 8690, 2024 04 15.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38622216

In the era of artificial intelligence, privacy empowerment illusion has become a crucial means for digital enterprises and platforms to "manipulate" users and create an illusion of control. This topic has also become an urgent and pressing concern for current research. However, the existing studies are limited in terms of their perspectives and methodologies, making it challenging to fully explain why users express concerns about privacy empowerment illusion but repeatedly disclose their personal information. This study combines the associative-propositional evaluation model (APE) and cognitive load theory, using event-related potential (ERP) technology to investigate the underlying mechanisms of how the comprehensibility and interpretability of privacy empowerment illusion cues affect users' immediate attitudes and privacy disclosure behaviours; these mechanisms are mediated by psychological processing and cognitive load differences. Behavioural research results indicate that in the context of privacy empowerment illusion cues with low comprehensibility, users are more inclined to disclose their private information when faced with high interpretability than they are when faced with low interpretability. EEG results show that in the context of privacy empowerment illusion cues with low comprehensibility, high interpretability induces greater P2 amplitudes than does low interpretability; low interpretability induces greater N2 amplitudes than does high interpretability. This study extends the scopes of the APE model and cognitive load theory in the field of privacy research, providing new insights into privacy attitudes. Doing so offers a valuable framework through which digital enterprises can gain a deeper understanding of users' genuine privacy attitudes and immediate reactions under privacy empowerment illusion situations. This understanding can help increase user privacy protection and improve their overall online experience, making it highly relevant and beneficial.


Hominidae , Illusions , Humans , Animals , Privacy/psychology , Disclosure , Cues , Artificial Intelligence , Cognition
8.
PLoS One ; 19(4): e0297850, 2024.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38625848

Power can increase overconfidence and illusory thinking. We investigated whether power is also related to the illusion of explanatory depth (IOED), people's tendency to think they understand the world in more detail, coherence, and depth than they actually do. Abstract thinking was reported as a reason for the IOED, and according to the social distance theory of power, power increases abstract thinking. We linked these literatures and tested construal style as a mediator. Further, predispositions can moderate effects of power and we considered narcissism as a candidate because narcissism leads to overconfidence and may thus increase the IOED especially in combination with high power. In three preregistered studies (total N = 607), we manipulated power or measured feelings of power. We found evidence for the IOED (regarding explanatory knowledge about devices). Power led to general overconfidence but had only a small impact on the IOED. Power and narcissism had a small interactive effect on the IOED. Meta-analytical techniques suggest that previous findings on the construal-style-IOED link show only weak evidential value. Implications refer to research on management, power, and overconfidence.


Illusions , Humans , Cognition , Thinking , Emotions , Surveys and Questionnaires
9.
Curr Biol ; 34(8): 1801-1809.e4, 2024 Apr 22.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38569544

Neural oscillations reflect fluctuations in the relative excitation/inhibition of neural systems1,2,3,4,5 and are theorized to play a critical role in canonical neural computations6,7,8,9 and cognitive processes.10,11,12,13,14 These theories have been supported by findings that detection of visual stimuli fluctuates with the phase of oscillations prior to stimulus onset.15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23 However, null results have emerged in studies seeking to demonstrate these effects in visual discrimination tasks,24,25,26,27 raising questions about the generalizability of these phenomena to wider neural processes. Recently, we suggested that methodological limitations may mask effects of phase in higher-level sensory processing.28 To test the generality of phasic influences on perception requires a task that involves stimulus discrimination while also depending on early sensory processing. Here, we examined the influence of oscillation phase on the visual tilt illusion, in which a center grating has its perceived orientation biased away from the orientation of a surround grating29 due to lateral inhibitory interactions in early visual processing.30,31,32 We presented center gratings at participants' subjective vertical angle and had participants report whether the grating appeared tilted clockwise or counterclockwise from vertical on each trial while measuring their brain activity with electroencephalography (EEG). In addition to effects of alpha power and aperiodic slope, we observed robust associations between orientation perception and alpha and theta phase, consistent with fluctuating illusion magnitude across the oscillatory cycle. These results confirm that oscillation phase affects the complex processing involved in stimulus discrimination, consistent with its purported role in canonical computations that underpin cognition.


Visual Perception , Humans , Male , Adult , Female , Visual Perception/physiology , Young Adult , Illusions/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Electroencephalography , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology
10.
Medicina (Kaunas) ; 60(4)2024 Apr 06.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38674252

Background and Objectives: Parkinson's disease (PD) is associated with various non-motor symptoms, including minor hallucinations, comprising visual illusions and presence and passage hallucinations. Despite their occurrence, even in newly diagnosed PD patients, data regarding the prevalence and characteristics of minor hallucinations, visual illusions in particular, remain limited. The aim of this study was to address this knowledge gap by assessing the prevalence of minor hallucinations in PD patients, with a focus on visual illusions. Materials and Methods: In this prospective pilot study, we enrolled 35 PD patients without dementia and 35 age- and gender-matched PD-unaffected individuals. Cognitive function was assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment, clinical data were collected, and all subjects were assessed via questionnaires regarding 20 types of visual illusions and other minor hallucinations. Results: The prevalence of minor hallucinations was significantly higher among PD patients compared to controls (45.7% vs. 11.4%, p = 0.003). PD patients reported visual illusions and presence hallucinations more frequently than the controls (37.1% vs. 8.6% and 22.9% vs. 2.9%, p = 0.009 and p = 0.028, respectively), with no significant difference in passage hallucinations (20% vs. 8.6%, p = 0.306). In the PD group, the most frequently observed visual illusions were complex visual illusions, kinetopsia, and pelopsia; the latter was also the most common visual illusion in the control group. PD patients experiencing visual illusions were more likely to report presence hallucinations compared to patients without visual illusions (53.8% vs. 4.5%, p = 0.002); no significant differences in other clinical characteristics were found. Conclusions: Minor hallucinations are a common phenomenon among PD patients without dementia, with a higher prevalence than among healthy controls. Visual illusions are the most prevalent type of minor hallucinations, affecting more than a third of PD patients, with complex visual illusions, kinetopsia, and pelopsia being the most frequently reported types.


Hallucinations , Illusions , Parkinson Disease , Humans , Parkinson Disease/complications , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Parkinson Disease/epidemiology , Hallucinations/epidemiology , Hallucinations/etiology , Female , Male , Lithuania/epidemiology , Aged , Prospective Studies , Illusions/physiology , Illusions/psychology , Middle Aged , Pilot Projects , Prevalence , Surveys and Questionnaires
11.
Rev Clin Esp (Barc) ; 224(5): 334-335, 2024 May.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38582349
12.
Conscious Cogn ; 121: 103684, 2024 May.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38613994

To what degree human cognition is influenced by subliminal stimuli is a controversial empirical question. One striking example was reported by Linser and Goschke (2007): participants overestimated how much control they had over objectively uncontrollable stimuli when masked congruent primes were presented immediately before the action. Critically, however, unawareness of the masked primes was established by post hoc data selection. In our preregistered study we sought to explore these findings while adjusting prime visibility based on individual thresholds, so that each participant underwent both visible and non-visible conditions. In experiment 1, N = 39 participants engaged in a control judgement task: following the presentation of a semantic prime, they freely selected between two keys, which triggered the appearance of a colored circle. The color of the circles, however, was independent of the key-press. Subsequently, participants assessed their perceived control over the circle's color, based on their key-presses, via a rating scale that ranged from 0 % (no control) to 100 % (complete control). Contrary to Linser and Goschke (2007)'s findings, this experiment demonstrated that predictive information influenced the experience of agency only when primes were consciously processed. In experiment 2, utilizing symbolic (arrow) primes, N = 35 participants had to rate their feeling of control over the effect-stimulus' identity during a two-choice identification paradigm (i.e., they were instructed to press a key corresponding to a target stimulus; with a contingency between target and effect stimulus of 75 %/25 %). The results revealed no significant influence of subliminal priming on agency perceptions. In summary, this study implies that unconscious stimuli may not exert a substantial influence on the conscious experience of agency, underscoring the need for careful consideration of methodological aspects and experimental design's impact on observed phenomena.


Unconscious, Psychology , Humans , Female , Male , Adult , Young Adult , Subliminal Stimulation , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Repetition Priming/physiology , Illusions/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adolescent , Awareness/physiology
13.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 45(4): e26653, 2024 Mar.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38488460

Face-to-face communication relies on the integration of acoustic speech signals with the corresponding facial articulations. In the McGurk illusion, an auditory /ba/ phoneme presented simultaneously with a facial articulation of a /ga/ (i.e., viseme), is typically fused into an illusory 'da' percept. Despite its widespread use as an index of audiovisual speech integration, critics argue that it arises from perceptual processes that differ categorically from natural speech recognition. Conversely, Bayesian theoretical frameworks suggest that both the illusory McGurk and the veridical audiovisual congruent speech percepts result from probabilistic inference based on noisy sensory signals. According to these models, the inter-sensory conflict in McGurk stimuli may only increase observers' perceptual uncertainty. This functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study presented participants (20 male and 24 female) with audiovisual congruent, McGurk (i.e., auditory /ba/ + visual /ga/), and incongruent (i.e., auditory /ga/ + visual /ba/) stimuli along with their unisensory counterparts in a syllable categorization task. Behaviorally, observers' response entropy was greater for McGurk compared to congruent audiovisual stimuli. At the neural level, McGurk stimuli increased activations in a widespread neural system, extending from the inferior frontal sulci (IFS) to the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) and insulae, typically involved in cognitive control processes. Crucially, in line with Bayesian theories these activation increases were fully accounted for by observers' perceptual uncertainty as measured by their response entropy. Our findings suggest that McGurk and congruent speech processing rely on shared neural mechanisms, thereby supporting the McGurk illusion as a valid measure of natural audiovisual speech perception.


Illusions , Speech Perception , Humans , Male , Female , Auditory Perception/physiology , Speech/physiology , Illusions/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Bayes Theorem , Uncertainty , Speech Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Photic Stimulation/methods
14.
Curr Biol ; 34(6): R229-R231, 2024 Mar 25.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38531310

Zhu et al. present an alternative explanation for the weaker multisensory illusions in football goalkeepers compared with outfielders and non-athletes, showing that better unisensory precision in goalkeepers can also account for this effect.


Auditory Perception , Illusions , Humans , Visual Perception , Acoustic Stimulation , Reaction Time , Photic Stimulation
15.
Curr Biol ; 34(6): R235-R236, 2024 Mar 25.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38531313

An important task for the visual system is to identify and segregate objects from background. Figure-ground illusions, such as Edgar Rubin's bistable 'vase-faces illusion'1, make the point clearly: we see either a central vase or lateral faces, alternating spontaneously, but never both images simultaneously. The border is perceptually assigned to either faces or vase, which become figure, the other shapeless background2. The stochastic alternation between figure and ground probably reflects mutual inhibitory processes that ensure a single perceptual outcome3. Which shape dominates perception depends on many factors, such as size, symmetry, convexity, enclosure, and so on, as well as attention and intention4. Here we show that the assignment of the visual border can be strongly influenced by auditory input, far more than is possible by voluntary intention. VIDEO ABSTRACT.


Illusions , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods , Attention , Face
16.
Cortex ; 174: 125-136, 2024 May.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38520766

Illusory neuropsychiatric symptoms such as hallucinations or the feeling of a presence (FOP) can occur in diffuse brain lesion or dysfunction, in psychiatric diseases as well as in healthy individuals. Their occurrence due to focal brain lesions is rare, most probably due to underreporting, which limits progress in understanding their underlying mechanisms and anatomical determinants. In this single case study, an 86-year-old patient experienced, in the context of an acute right central opercular ischemic stroke, visual hallucinatory symptoms (including palinopsia), differently lateralized auditory hallucinations and FOP. This unusual clinical constellation could be precisely documented and illustrated while still present, allowing a realistic and immersive visual experience validated by the patient. The acute stroke appeared to be their most plausible cause (after exclusion of other etiologies). Furthermore, accurate analysis of tractographic data suggested that disruption in the posterior bundle of the superior longitudinal fasciculus connecting the stroke lesion to the inferior parietal lobule was the anatomical substrate explaining the FOP and, indirectly, also hallucinations through whiter matter involvement, in coherence with existing literature. We could finally elaborate on symptoms taxonomy and phenomenology (e.g., polyopic heautoscopy, hallucinatory FOP, etc), and on patient's remarkable distancing from them (with some therapeutic implications supported by plausibly engaged mechanisms). This case not only authentically enriched the description of such rare combination of heterogenous illusory symptoms through this novel visualization-based reporting approach, but disclosed an unrevealed anatomo-clinical link relating all of them to the acute stroke lesion through an association fiber, thereby contributing to the understanding of these intriguing symptoms and their determinants.


Illusions , Perceptual Disorders , Stroke , Vision Disorders , Humans , Aged, 80 and over , Hallucinations , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Stroke/complications , Stroke/diagnostic imaging
17.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 6434, 2024 03 18.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38499578

Perceptual grouping is impaired following mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI). This may affect visual size perception, a process influenced by perceptual grouping abilities. We conducted two experiments to evaluate visual size perception in people with self-reported history of mTBI, using two different size-contrast illusions: the Ebbinghaus Illusion (Experiment 1) and the Müller-Lyer illusion (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, individuals with mTBI and healthy controls were asked to compare the size of two target circles that were either the same size or different sizes. The target circles appeared by themselves (no-context condition), or were surrounded by smaller or larger circles (context condition). Similar levels of accuracy were evident between the groups in the no-context condition. However, size judgements by mTBI participants were more accurate in the context condition, suggesting that they processed the target circles separately from the surrounding circles. In Experiment 2, individuals with mTBI and healthy controls judged the length of parallel lines that appeared with arrowheads (context condition) or without arrowheads (no context condition). Consistent with Experiment 1, size judgements by mTBI participants were more accurate than size judgements by control participants in the context condition. These findings suggest that mTBI influences size perception by impairing perceptual grouping of visual stimuli in near proximity.


Brain Concussion , Illusions , Optical Illusions , Humans , Visual Perception , Size Perception , Judgment
18.
Comput Methods Programs Biomed ; 248: 108124, 2024 May.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38503070

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: Many recent studies in virtual reality (VR) have managed the sense of Presence to assess the suitability of their designs, mainly when focused on learning goals that require high user engagement, such as in serious games for psychomotor training. However, the place and plausibility illusions needed to promote Presence are achieved by combining different VR-based design cues, and their individual contribution to preserving the Presence's engagement/involvement component is still unclear. This article explored the single effect of breaking the sense of Presence per VR factor, i.e., removing VR cues related to Social Presence (human interactions), Self-Presence (embodiment), and Physical Presence (Scenario realism). METHODS: Thirty-three participants were asked to play an immersive VR simulation of an arcade game three times by experiencing a stepped Break of Illusion in one of the VR factors, i.e., while two factors were kept high, the remaining one was reduced to a low and null (hypothetical) level. The game difficulty was fixed after assessing each person's skills. RESULTS: Results showed that psychophysiology indicators (heart rate and skin conductance) were not affected by the level of illusion, whereas exercise intensity was significantly higher with low body and social presence-based conditions. Moreover, skin conductance was lower in the Social-presence group, which suggests that perspiration is only affected by breaks in realism (scenario and body representations). Based on the obtained evidence, we proposed some guidelines for adapting the design of immersive virtual environments through Breaks in Presence, mainly by changing the realism of the scenario and body representation depending on the skin conductance or the interaction with virtual humans depending on exercise intensity.


Illusions , Virtual Reality , Humans , Computer Simulation , Heart Rate , Learning
20.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(12): e2315758121, 2024 Mar 19.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38489383

Grid cells in the entorhinal cortex (EC) encode an individual's location in space, integrating both environmental and multisensory bodily cues. Notably, body-derived signals are also primary signals for the sense of self. While studies have demonstrated that continuous application of visuo-tactile bodily stimuli can induce perceptual shifts in self-location, it remains unexplored whether these illusory changes suffice to trigger grid cell-like representation (GCLR) within the EC, and how this compares to GCLR during conventional virtual navigation. To address this, we systematically induced illusory drifts in self-location toward controlled directions using visuo-tactile bodily stimulation, while maintaining the subjects' visual viewpoint fixed (absent conventional virtual navigation). Subsequently, we evaluated the corresponding GCLR in the EC through functional MRI analysis. Our results reveal that illusory changes in perceived self-location (independent of changes in environmental navigation cues) can indeed evoke entorhinal GCLR, correlating in strength with the magnitude of perceived self-location, and characterized by similar grid orientation as during conventional virtual navigation in the same virtual room. These data demonstrate that the same grid-like representation is recruited when navigating based on environmental, mainly visual cues, or when experiencing illusory forward drifts in self-location, driven by perceptual multisensory bodily cues.


Grid Cells , Illusions , Spatial Navigation , Humans , Entorhinal Cortex/physiology , Grid Cells/physiology , Consciousness , Illusions/physiology , Touch , Spatial Navigation/physiology
...